Saussure introduces "arbitrariness" twice:
One can start an exploration of this idea in Saussure's treatment of, first, the relationship of words to objects as it seemed at the time our brains might apprehend them.[ftn01]
The first time Saussure writes about "arbitrariness,' he does so in the context of the relationship between the signifier and the signified.
p. 61
This discussion appears to do two things that are easily shown to be unworkable as taken by themselves
However the opening pages on the sign must precisely not be lifted out of the total text and made to stand on their own.
First, one must understand how the analysis of the sign can be used in phonology where signifying phonemes are built by bringing together in an altogether arbitrary fashions various articulatory or graphic possibilities to make distinctions on which meaningful syntagms (in this case 'words') can be built.
Second, one must pay close attention to the discussion of "linguistic value" in Chapter IV.
Saussure briefly sketches a powerful argument summarized in a simple statement and a diagram.
Without language thought is a vague, uncharted nebula. There are no pre-existing ideas, and nothing is distinct before the appearance of language. (p. 112)
Not only does this discussion provide the foundation for all (post-)structuralisms, but it allows for the fundamental convergence of American cultural anthropology with Saussure on which Lévi-Strauss insisted but which was mostly lost in the symbolic anthropologies that were built over Geertz's critique of Lévi-Strauss.
In the diagram, 'A' represents the "uncharted nebula" of pre-linguistic thought. 'B' represents the continuum of vocal possibilities presented to human beings by their biology (the range is even broader if one adds all visual possibilities to the auditory ones). The vertical lines represent the arbitrary distinctions that are made in both continua and their relationships both within and across what we might talk as various 'media' (it may not be literally Saussurian to think about thought or the brain as a medium, but recent development about human beings as 'cyborgs' might allow this)
p.113
The best and most classic example of this is the analysis of human physicical perception of light (which must be that of a continuum) in relation to color vocabularies that segment the 'rainbow' into any number of colors.
Violet, Indigo, Blue, Green, Yellow, Orange, and Red ???
From Boas onwards cultural anthropologists have emphasized the various ways that languages around the world use to express this rainbow in their vocabularies.
There has been serious controversies about the extent to which color vocabularies can be said to be "arbitrary." The central argument against simple relativism was made by Berlin and Kay (1969). There is every evidence that human beings all perceive color in the same way (which would relate to what the Boasian referred to as the "psychic unity of manking") and that there is an ordering in the development of more complex color vocabularies. The state of the controversy is summarized by Nicola Jones (2017).
The simple Saussurian statement of semiotic arbitrariness however raises two sets of issues:
On the way to addressing these issues, we must first consider semiotic arbitrariness in less stereotyped contexts than color vocabularies.
ftn01: Of course this initial statement was radically criticized later, starting with Derrida ().